Poster No:
2004
Submission Type:
Abstract Submission
Authors:
Yaron Caspi1, Dimitar Ivanov2, Grace Hung1, Yiling Tseng3, Likai Huang4, Tzu-Yu Hsu5, Philip Tseng1, Po-Jang (Brown) Hsieh1
Institutions:
1Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan, 2College of Natural Sciences, Minerva University, San Francisco, CA, 3Department of Clinical Psychology, Fu Jen Catholic University, New Taipei City, Taiwan, 4Dementia Center, Department of Neurology, Shuang Ho Hospital, Taipei Medical University, New Taipei City, Taiwan, 5Graduate Institute of Mind, Brain, and Consciousness, Taipei Medical University, Taipei, Taiwan
First Author:
Yaron Caspi
Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University
Taipei, Taiwan
Co-Author(s):
Dimitar Ivanov
College of Natural Sciences, Minerva University
San Francisco, CA
Grace Hung
Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University
Taipei, Taiwan
Yiling Tseng
Department of Clinical Psychology, Fu Jen Catholic University
New Taipei City, Taiwan
Likai Huang
Dementia Center, Department of Neurology, Shuang Ho Hospital, Taipei Medical University
New Taipei City, Taiwan
Tzu-Yu Hsu
Graduate Institute of Mind, Brain, and Consciousness, Taipei Medical University
Taipei, Taiwan
Philip Tseng
Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University
Taipei, Taiwan
Introduction:
Brain activity, which results in behavior, is the product of cellular activity. Underlying the neuronal and glial cells' activities are fundamental biological processes executed by these cells' biological machinery. Hence, a logical question to ask is - are there biological processes and pathways that are correlated with a specific observable behavior?
Methods:
To answer this question, we followed the methodology from our previous publication (see (Tan et al., 2019)). We studied two commonly used experimental paradigms, namely, Attentional Blink (for attention) (Hommel et al., 2006) and Change Detection (for working memory) (Luck & Vogel, 2013). We used the expression data from the Allen Brain Human Atlas (Hawrylycz et al., 2012) together with self-collected published evidence about fMRI brain activity during the execution of these paradigms (see Fig. 1). As a control, we repeated the same analysis for data of specific functions that were obtained from the Neurosytnh and NeuroQuery databases (motor imagery, executive functions, metacognition, and visual word recognition).
Expression levels were obtained using ABAGEN (Markello et al., 2021). We used the meta-analysis NiAMARE tool (Salo et al., 2022) on the curated fMRI data to extract the Z-values related to the fMRI activity. We parceled the resulted Z-value and expression data using brain atlases for the cortical, subcortical, and cerebellar regions (cortical – (Kong et al., 2021) (aka Shaffaer100 and Shaffaer1000); subcortical – (Tian et al., 2020); cerebellum – (Buckner et al., 2011) and (Nettekoven et al., 2024)).
To identify pathways associated with Attentional Blink and Change Detection, we run a linear correlation analysis between the parceled RNA levels and the parceled fMRI Z-values. This analysis resulted in a list of genes and the associated correlation t-values for each of the two paradigms and for each brain region. Next, we used pathways-enrichment-based tools (g:Profiler (Kolberg et al., 2023), GSEA (Subramanian et al., 2005), and EnrichmentMap (Reimand et al., 2019)) to identify biological pathways that are enriched among the genes mostly correlated with the fMRI Z-values for each of the studied conditions.
We also collected published data about brain stimulation that influenced the execution of these two experimental paradigms. Next, we used a simulation tool (SimNIBS) (Thielscher et al., 2015) to simulate the expected electric field in the brain from published transcranial Magnetic and transcranial Direct Current stimulation experiments (TMS and tDCS). Finally, we repeated the same pipeline described above to deduce biological pathways associated with the stimulated brain regions.

Results:
After filtering out housekeeping pathways, we studied pathways that were statistically significantly correlated with the brain activity pattern of the two paradigms (see Fig. 2). For the left cortical hemisphere, we identified the ERK1/ERK2 pathway which was implicated in glial progenitor cell, as one that is associated with Attentional Blink, but not with Change Blindness (Nevertheless, it was implicated, though to a lesser extent in some control conditions). By contrast, Dopamine signaling was unique to the Change Detection paradigm. For the subcortical regions, we identify clathrin-coated pits and cilium-related functions as two pathways that were repeatedly implicated in the studied fMRI foci maps.
Conclusions:
We identified several candidate pathways correlating with brain activity for an attentional blink or change detection in specific brain regions (cortex, sub-cortex, or cerebellum). In some cases, pathways were unique or more strongly correlated with specific fMRI patterns. In other cases, some pathways were not unique for specific paradigms (control paradigms also implicated these pathways). Yet, pathways from the second group are not ones that are naively expected for behavioral-dependent brain activity. Hence, they might point to an interesting future research direction.
Genetics:
Transcriptomics
Genetics Other 2
Perception, Attention and Motor Behavior:
Attention: Visual 1
Consciousness and Awareness
Keywords:
Cerebellum
Consciousness
Cortex
FUNCTIONAL MRI
Perception
Phenotype-Genotype
Sub-Cortical
Vision
Other - Gene Expression; Working Memory;
1|2Indicates the priority used for review
By submitting your proposal, you grant permission for the Organization for Human Brain Mapping (OHBM) to distribute your work in any format, including video, audio print and electronic text through OHBM OnDemand, social media channels, the OHBM website, or other electronic publications and media.
I accept
The Open Science Special Interest Group (OSSIG) is introducing a reproducibility challenge for OHBM 2025. This new initiative aims to enhance the reproducibility of scientific results and foster collaborations between labs. Teams will consist of a “source” party and a “reproducing” party, and will be evaluated on the success of their replication, the openness of the source work, and additional deliverables. Click here for more information.
Propose your OHBM abstract(s) as source work for future OHBM meetings by selecting one of the following options:
I am submitting this abstract as an original work to be reproduced. I am available to be the “source party” in an upcoming team and consent to have this work listed on the OSSIG website. I agree to be contacted by OSSIG regarding the challenge and may share data used in this abstract with another team.
Please indicate below if your study was a "resting state" or "task-activation” study.
Other
Healthy subjects only or patients (note that patient studies may also involve healthy subjects):
Healthy subjects
Was this research conducted in the United States?
No
Were any human subjects research approved by the relevant Institutional Review Board or ethics panel?
NOTE: Any human subjects studies without IRB approval will be automatically rejected.
Not applicable
Were any animal research approved by the relevant IACUC or other animal research panel?
NOTE: Any animal studies without IACUC approval will be automatically rejected.
Not applicable
Please indicate which methods were used in your research:
Computational modeling
Which processing packages did you use for your study?
Other, Please list
-
NiMARE, SimNiBS, ABAGEN
Provide references using APA citation style.
Buckner, R. L. … Yeo, B. T. T. (2011). The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106(5), 2322–2345.
Hawrylycz, M. J. … Jones, A. R. (2012). An anatomically comprehensive atlas of the adult human brain transcriptome. Nature, 489(7416), 391–399.
Hommel, B. … Schnitzler, A. (2006). How the brain blinks: Towards a neurocognitive model of the attentional blink. Psychological Research, 70(6), 425–435.
Kolberg, L. … Peterson, H. (2023). g:Profiler—Interoperable web service for functional enrichment analysis and gene identifier mapping (2023 update). Nucleic Acids Research, 51(W1),
Kong, R. … Yeo, B. T. T. (2021). Individual-Specific Areal-Level Parcellations Improve Functional Connectivity Prediction of Behavior. Cerebral Cortex, 31(10), 4477–4500.
Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (2013). Visual working memory capacity: From psychophysics and neurobiology to individual differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(8), 391–400.
Markello, R. D. … Misic, B. (2021). Standardizing workflows in imaging transcriptomics with the abagen toolbox. eLife, 10, e72129.
Nettekoven, C. … Diedrichsen, J. (2024). A hierarchical atlas of the human cerebellum for functional precision mapping. Nature Communications, 15(1), 8376.
Reimand, J. … Bader, G. D. (2019). Pathway enrichment analysis and visualization of omics data using g:Profiler, GSEA, Cytoscape and EnrichmentMap. Nature Protocols, 14(2), 482–517.
Salo, T. … Laird, A. R. (2022). neurostuff/NiMARE: 0.0.12rc7 (Version 0.0.12rc7) [Computer software]. Zenodo. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6642243
Subramanian, A. … Mesirov, J. P. (2005). Gene set enrichment analysis: A knowledge-based approach for interpreting genome-wide expression profiles. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(43), 15545–15550.
Tan, P. K. … Hsieh, P.-J. (2019). Distinct Genetic Signatures of Cortical and Subcortical Regions Associated with Human Memory. Eneuro, 6(6), ENEURO.0283-19.2019.
Thielscher, A. … Saturnino, G. B. (2015). Field modeling for transcranial magnetic stimulation: A useful tool to understand the physiological effects of TMS? 2015 37th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), 222–225.
Tian, Y. … Zalesky, A. (2020). Topographic organization of the human subcortex unveiled with functional connectivity gradients. Nature Neuroscience, 23(11), 1421–1432.
No