Neural Mechanisms of Phonological Interference and Facilitation in Spoken Word Production

Poster No:

823 

Submission Type:

Abstract Submission 

Authors:

Lydia Huang1, Katie McMahon2, Greig de Zubicaray1, Angélique Volfart3

Institutions:

1Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Queensland, 2Royal Brisbane & Women’s Hospital; Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Queensland, 3University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg, Kirchberg

First Author:

Lydia Huang  
Queensland University of Technology
Brisbane, Queensland

Co-Author(s):

Katie McMahon  
Royal Brisbane & Women’s Hospital; Queensland University of Technology
Brisbane, Queensland
Greig de Zubicaray  
Queensland University of Technology
Brisbane, Queensland
Angélique Volfart  
University of Luxembourg
Luxembourg, Kirchberg

Introduction:

Two brain areas – the left posterior superior temporal gyrus (LpSTG) and the left supramarginal gyrus (LSMG) – have been proposed to be associated with phonological processes in word production (Dell et al., 2013; Indefrey, 2011). Interestingly, the proposals were based on different sources of evidence: accurate picture naming in healthy participants for the LpSTG (e.g., de Zubicaray et al., 2002) and non-word speech errors made by lesion patients during picture naming for the LSMG (Dell et al., 2013). This suggests that the two brain areas may reflect engagement at different levels of phonological processing during production: lexical vs. post-lexical (articulatory phonetic).
Here, we aim to distinguish the functional roles of these two brain areas using a word production paradigm that elicits two distinct phonological effects with a phoneme-level manipulation. These two effects are facilitation (faster responses for words that share the first phoneme) and interference (slower responses for words sharing distributed phonemes). We hypothesized that the LpSTG is activated during the facilitation effect reflecting its role in lexical word form processing whereas the LSMG is engaged during the interference effect due to its role in post-lexical/articulatory phonetic processes.

Methods:

This multiband 3T fMRI study employed a blocked cyclic naming paradigm to elicit phonological facilitation and interference effects. Seventeen participants attended 2 sessions (3-13 days apart), during which they completed one task eliciting either facilitation or interference effect (task order was counterbalanced across sessions). Participants were presented with blocks of 6 pictures that were randomly presented 6 times (organized as cycles). Blocks were either homogeneous (picture names shared phonological features), or heterogeneous (picture names did not share phonological features). In the facilitation task, homogeneous blocks included picture names sharing the first phoneme. In the interference task, homogeneous blocks contained picture names with phonemic overlap distributed across word positions (Breining et al., 2016). The two tasks contained distinctive sets of pictures and names and were tested behaviourally prior to this fMRI study to ensure they elicited the expected effects (p < .001, η² > .43). In separate participant groups, the first phoneme overlap produced facilitation from the first cycle onward (mean difference = -19.97ms), and distributed phonemic overlap showed interference from the second cycle onwards (mean difference = 22.54ms).
Preprocessing was conducted with SPM12 (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12/) and the CONN toolbox (version 22.a) in MATLAB R2019B for each task, including removing artifacts from muscular movements, respiration, and CSF (Volfart et al., 2024). Task activation maps were then generated and contrasted (homogeneous vs. heterogeneous). All brain results were family-wise error corrected and labeled with Brainnetome (Fan et al., 2016).

Results:

Results support our hypothesis regarding the interference effect. Significant signal increases were observed in the left inferior frontal gyrus, the LSMG, the left fusiform gyrus, and the left ventrolateral premotor cortex in the homogeneous relative to the heterogeneous blocks. Significant signal decreases were observed in the right anterior cingulate cortex. As for the facilitation effect, no brain clusters have survived correction.
Supporting Image: table.png
Supporting Image: figure.png
 

Conclusions:

Our results suggest that the LSMG processes articulatory phonetic representations for word production, as it is engaged during the interference effect that arises from sequencing articulatory phonetic features in varying word positions. We did not observe significant activation in LpSTG or any other region during the facilitation effect, suggesting that the processes involved may be different and/or are more weakly engaged than those that have been found to activate the LpSTG in previous studies (e.g., de Zubicaray & McMahon, 2009).

Language:

Speech Production 1

Novel Imaging Acquisition Methods:

BOLD fMRI 2

Keywords:

FUNCTIONAL MRI
Language
Other - Phonology, Production, Blocked cyclic naming, Multiband fMRI

1|2Indicates the priority used for review

Abstract Information

By submitting your proposal, you grant permission for the Organization for Human Brain Mapping (OHBM) to distribute your work in any format, including video, audio print and electronic text through OHBM OnDemand, social media channels, the OHBM website, or other electronic publications and media.

I accept

The Open Science Special Interest Group (OSSIG) is introducing a reproducibility challenge for OHBM 2025. This new initiative aims to enhance the reproducibility of scientific results and foster collaborations between labs. Teams will consist of a “source” party and a “reproducing” party, and will be evaluated on the success of their replication, the openness of the source work, and additional deliverables. Click here for more information. Propose your OHBM abstract(s) as source work for future OHBM meetings by selecting one of the following options:

I am submitting this abstract as an original work to be reproduced. I am available to be the “source party” in an upcoming team and consent to have this work listed on the OSSIG website. I agree to be contacted by OSSIG regarding the challenge and may share data used in this abstract with another team.

Please indicate below if your study was a "resting state" or "task-activation” study.

Task-activation

Healthy subjects only or patients (note that patient studies may also involve healthy subjects):

Healthy subjects

Was this research conducted in the United States?

No

Were any human subjects research approved by the relevant Institutional Review Board or ethics panel? NOTE: Any human subjects studies without IRB approval will be automatically rejected.

Yes

Were any animal research approved by the relevant IACUC or other animal research panel? NOTE: Any animal studies without IACUC approval will be automatically rejected.

Not applicable

Please indicate which methods were used in your research:

Functional MRI

For human MRI, what field strength scanner do you use?

3.0T

Which processing packages did you use for your study?

SPM
Other, Please list  -   CONN, Macey

Provide references using APA citation style.

1. Breining, B., Nozari, N., & Rapp, B. (2016). Does segmental overlap help or hurt? Evidence from blocked cyclic naming in spoken and written production. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 500–506. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0900-x
2. de Zubicaray, G. I., & McMahon, K. L. (2009). Auditory context effects in picture naming investigated with event-related fMRI. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9(3), 260–269. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.9.3.260
3. de Zubicaray, G. I., McMahon, K. L., Eastburn, M., & Wilson, S. (2002). Orthographic/phonological facilitation of naming responses in the picture-word task: An event-related fMRI study using overt vocal responding. NeuroImage, 16(4), 1084–1093. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1135
4. Dell, G. S., Schwartz, M. F., Nozari, N., Faseyitan, O., & Branch Coslett, H. (2013). Voxel-based lesion-parameter mapping: Identifying the neural correlates of a computational model of word production. Cognition, 128(3), 380–396. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.05.007
5. Fan, L., Li, H., Zhuo, J., Zhang, Y., Wang, J., Chen, L., Yang, Z., Chu, C., Xie, S., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., Eickhoff, S. B., Yu, C., & Jiang, T. (2016). The human Brainnetome atlas: A new brain atlas based on connectional architecture. Cerebral Cortex, 26(8), 3508–3526. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw157
6. Indefrey, P. (2011). The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components: A critical update. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00255
7. Volfart, A., McMahon, K. L., & de Zubicaray, G. I. (2024). A comparison of denoising approaches for spoken word production related artefacts in continuous multiband fMRI data. Neurobiology of Language, 5(4), 901–921. https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00151

UNESCO Institute of Statistics and World Bank Waiver Form

I attest that I currently live, work, or study in a country on the UNESCO Institute of Statistics and World Bank List of Low and Middle Income Countries list provided.

No