Neural mechanisms underlying the relationship between inhibitory control and noradrenergic system

Poster No:

726 

Submission Type:

Abstract Submission 

Authors:

Marcela Díaz Díaz1, Maria Paz Martinez-Molina2, Patricia Soto-Icaza2, Alejandra Figueroa-Vargas1, Mariana Ayala-Ochoa2, Victor Marquez2, Paulo Figueroa-Taiba2, Tomás Ossandón1, Pablo Billeke2

Institutions:

1Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, 2Universidad del Desarrollo, Santiago, Chile

First Author:

Marcela Díaz Díaz  
Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile
Santiago, Chile

Co-Author(s):

Maria Paz Martinez-Molina  
Universidad del Desarrollo
Santiago, Chile
Patricia Soto-Icaza  
Universidad del Desarrollo
Santiago, Chile
Alejandra Figueroa-Vargas  
Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile
Santiago, Chile
Mariana Ayala-Ochoa  
Universidad del Desarrollo
Santiago, Chile
Victor Marquez  
Universidad del Desarrollo
Santiago, Chile
Paulo Figueroa-Taiba  
Universidad del Desarrollo
Santiago, Chile
Tomás Ossandón  
Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile
Santiago, Chile
Pablo Billeke  
Universidad del Desarrollo
Santiago, Chile

Introduction:

In daily life, successful goal-directed behavior depends on our ability to inhibit impulses or distractions (Diamond, 2013). When facing conflictive situations where automatic responses must be suppressed, conflict expectation emerges (Aron, 2010), increasing cognitive control demands.
The Go-NoGo paradigm is widely employed to study inhibitory control. Increasing the number of consecutive Go stimuli before a NoGo stimulus enhances conflict expectation, leading to longer reaction times (Zamorano et al., 2014). Fronto-medial theta oscillations, especially in the superior frontal gyrus (SFG), have been linked to conflict anticipation and its effects on performance (Dippel et al., 2017). Martinez-Molina et al. (2024) further showed that theta-band rhythmic stimulation intensifies these effects, increasing RTs and improving NoGo accuracy. Additionally, the locus coeruleus-noradrenergic (LC-NE) system supports cognitive control and attention. Its activity, measured indirectly via pupil diameter, increase with cognitive demands (Pfeffer et al., 2022), and its integrity correlates with better inhibitory performance (Tomassini et al., 2022). Prior evidence indicates that as NoGo stimuli become less frequent-thus increasing conflict-theta power and pupil diameter both rise (Dippel, 2017). In this study, we aim to clarify the neural mechanisms that link conflict expectation, inhibitory control, and the LC-NE system by examining how changes in pupil dynamics relate to fronto-medial theta oscillations.

Methods:

Thirty-two healthy participants (19 women; age range: 18-45 years, mean: 27 ± 7 years) performed a modified Go-NoGo task manipulating the number of consecutive Go trials before a NoGo to modulate conflict expectation. Behavioral data (RTs), electroencephalography (EEG), and pupillometry were recorded. We applied an integrative approach, combining cognitive computational modeling (Bayesian hierarchical drift-diffusion model), EEG time-frequency analyses, and pupil dilation indices to assess the interplay of conflict expectation and inhibitory control. Statistical comparisons on EEG and behavioral measures were conducted using non-parametric Wilcoxon tests with Bonferroni correction. A linear mixed-effects (LME) model was used to analyze pupil dilation, incorporating conflict expectation and previous error as fixed factors.

Results:

Behaviorally, the Bayesian drift-diffusion model indicated a significant effect of conflict expectation on RTs (β = 52.3, p < 0.001), suggesting that higher conflict anticipation increases cognitive control demands. For pupil dilation, the LME model (N=13,743 observations) revealed significant positive effects of conflict expectation (β = 5.91, p = 0.0389) and previous error (perror; β = 4.26, p = 0.00022) on pupil size, indicating that both increased conflict expectation and recent errors increase the LC-NE system engagement (Figure 1). Time-frequency EEG analyses focused on fronto-medial electrodes showed that conditions associated with higher conflict expectation modulated theta power. Non-parametric Wilcoxon tests with Bonferroni correction confirmed these differences, aligning with the notion that theta oscillations track inhibitory control needs (Figure 2).
Supporting Image: Figure1.png
Supporting Image: Figure2.png
 

Conclusions:

Participants adapt their behavior in anticipation of conflict, evidenced by slower responses when expecting challenging tasks demands. This adaptive mechanism suggests that cognitive resources are dynamically allocated based on increasing expectations, engaging inhibitory control processes before the presentation of a conflicting stimulus. In addition, the modulation of frontal theta activity in the right lateral prefrontal cortex, together with changes in pupil dilation, highlights the interplay between these neural markers and the LC-NE system in shaping responses to conflict expectation. These results provide new insights into the underlying brain mechanisms that link inhibitory control and the noradrenergic system in the context of conflict expectation.

Higher Cognitive Functions:

Executive Function, Cognitive Control and Decision Making 1

Modeling and Analysis Methods:

Bayesian Modeling
EEG/MEG Modeling and Analysis 2

Keywords:

Other - Inhibitory control, conflict expectation, EEG, pupillometry, computational modeling, theta oscillations, Locus coeruleus - norepinephrine system

1|2Indicates the priority used for review

Abstract Information

By submitting your proposal, you grant permission for the Organization for Human Brain Mapping (OHBM) to distribute your work in any format, including video, audio print and electronic text through OHBM OnDemand, social media channels, the OHBM website, or other electronic publications and media.

I accept

The Open Science Special Interest Group (OSSIG) is introducing a reproducibility challenge for OHBM 2025. This new initiative aims to enhance the reproducibility of scientific results and foster collaborations between labs. Teams will consist of a “source” party and a “reproducing” party, and will be evaluated on the success of their replication, the openness of the source work, and additional deliverables. Click here for more information. Propose your OHBM abstract(s) as source work for future OHBM meetings by selecting one of the following options:

I am submitting this abstract as an original work to be reproduced. I am available to be the “source party” in an upcoming team and consent to have this work listed on the OSSIG website. I agree to be contacted by OSSIG regarding the challenge and may share data used in this abstract with another team.

Please indicate below if your study was a "resting state" or "task-activation” study.

Task-activation

Healthy subjects only or patients (note that patient studies may also involve healthy subjects):

Healthy subjects

Was this research conducted in the United States?

No

Were any human subjects research approved by the relevant Institutional Review Board or ethics panel? NOTE: Any human subjects studies without IRB approval will be automatically rejected.

Yes

Were any animal research approved by the relevant IACUC or other animal research panel? NOTE: Any animal studies without IACUC approval will be automatically rejected.

Not applicable

Please indicate which methods were used in your research:

EEG/ERP
Behavior
Other, Please specify  -   Pupillometry

Provide references using APA citation style.

Aron, A. R. (2011). From reactive to proactive and selective control: developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biological psychiatry, 69(12), e55-e68.

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. In Annual Review of Psychology (Vol. 64, pp. 135–168). Annual Reviews Inc.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

Dippel, G., Mückschel, M., Ziemssen, T., & Beste, C. (2017). Demands on response inhibition processes determine modulations of theta band activity in superior frontal areas and correlations with pupillometry – Implications for the norepinephrine system during inhibitory control. NeuroImage, 157, 575–585. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.037

Martínez-Molina, M. P., Valdebenito-Oyarzo, G., Soto-Icaza, P., Zamorano, F., Figueroa-Vargas, A., Carvajal-Paredes, P., Stecher, X., Salinas, C., Valero-Cabré, A., Polania, R., & Billeke, P. (2024). Lateral Prefrontal Theta Oscillations Causally Drive a Computational Mechanism Underlying Conflict Expectation and Adaptation. https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.04.30.591918

Pfeffer, T., Keitel, C., Kluger, D. S., Keitel, A., Russmann, A., Thut, G., Donner, T. H., & Gross, J. (2022). Coupling of pupil-and neuronal population dynamics reveals diverse influences of arousal on cortical processing. ELife, 11. https://doi.org/10.7554/ELIFE.71890

Tomassini, A., Hezemans, F. H., Ye, R., Tsvetanov, K. A., Wolpe, N., & Rowe, J. B. (2022). Prefrontal cortical connectivity mediates locus coeruleus noradrenergic regulation of inhibitory control in older adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 42(16), 3484-3493.

Zamorano, F., Billeke, P., Hurtado, J. M., López, V., Carrasco, X., Ossandón, T., & Aboitiz, F. (2014). Temporal constraints of behavioral inhibition: Relevance of inter-stimulus interval in a go-nogo task. PLoS ONE, 9(1). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0087232

UNESCO Institute of Statistics and World Bank Waiver Form

I attest that I currently live, work, or study in a country on the UNESCO Institute of Statistics and World Bank List of Low and Middle Income Countries list provided.

No