Neural Mechanisms of Social Network Expansion: Based On Egocentric and Allocentric Perspective

Poster No:

1886 

Submission Type:

Abstract Submission 

Authors:

Huihui Niu1, Taihan Chen1, Wenzhao Deng1, Zhizhong Jiang1, Hongyimei Liu1, Zhongyin Liang1, Tiantian Liu1, Xiaoyan Wu2, Ruiwang Huang1

Institutions:

1School of Psychology, Key Laboratory of Brain, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, Guangdong, 2School of Psychology, Fujian Normal University, Fuzhou, Fujian

First Author:

Huihui Niu  
School of Psychology, Key Laboratory of Brain, South China Normal University
Guangzhou, Guangdong

Co-Author(s):

Taihan Chen  
School of Psychology, Key Laboratory of Brain, South China Normal University
Guangzhou, Guangdong
Wenzhao Deng  
School of Psychology, Key Laboratory of Brain, South China Normal University
Guangzhou, Guangdong
Zhizhong Jiang  
School of Psychology, Key Laboratory of Brain, South China Normal University
Guangzhou, Guangdong
Hongyimei Liu  
School of Psychology, Key Laboratory of Brain, South China Normal University
Guangzhou, Guangdong
Zhongyin Liang  
School of Psychology, Key Laboratory of Brain, South China Normal University
Guangzhou, Guangdong
Tiantian Liu  
School of Psychology, Key Laboratory of Brain, South China Normal University
Guangzhou, Guangdong
Xiaoyan Wu  
School of Psychology, Fujian Normal University
Fuzhou, Fujian
Ruiwang Huang  
School of Psychology, Key Laboratory of Brain, South China Normal University
Guangzhou, Guangdong

Introduction:

Social networks are structured representations of relationships and interaction patterns among individuals. Social Network Analysis (SNA) is a method for quantifying interactions between individuals (Butts, 2008). In social networks, the relational representation involves two types of reference frame (RF): egocentric RF, which encodes self-other relationships from the self's perspective, and allocentric RF, which encodes relationships among others from a third-party perspective (Epstein et al., 2017). Study showed that encountering others in one's social network activates brain regions encoding social knowledge, like the mentalizing network (Schwyck et al., 2023). Social network expansion is also influenced by factors such as similarity and popularity (Harris & Vazire, 2016; Van Den Broek et al., 2016). Previous studies on neural encoding of social networks mainly focus on egocentric representations, with brain regions such as the inferior and superior parietal lobes, superior temporal gyrus, medial prefrontal cortex, and temporoparietal junction implicated. (Parkinson et al., 2017; Peer et al., 2021). However, it remains unclear whether the allocentric RF influences the social networks and what the neural mechanisms underlying allocentric representations. The current study aims to reveal the mechanisms of reference frame selection for social network expansion using an fMRI experiment with a social relationship selection task.

Methods:

Subjects. We recruited 33 healthy adult subjects from South China Normal University (SCNU). A dataset was excluded due to accuracy ≤ 85% in a memory test after watching the movie. A total of 32 subjects (17M/15F, age = 20.81 ± 2.46 years) were included in the following analysis. The study was approved by the IRB of SCNU. Written informed consent was obtained from each subject.
Data acquisition and preprocessing. The MRI data was acquired on a 3.0 T Siemens Prismafit MR scanner equipped with a 64-channel phased-array head coil. Both anatomical and functional MRI data were preprocessed using fMRIprep (Ver 20.2.7). The preprocessing steps were as follows: (1) smoothing with a 6-mm FWHM Gaussian kernal, and (2) high-pass filtering at 1/128 Hz to eliminate the low-frequency artifacts.
Experiment design and procedures. Figs. 1a and 1b illustrate the experimental procedure. The subjects first engaged in learning a new social network and measured the closeness between the characters. Then, they performed the social relationship selection task during the fMRI scan.
Whole-brain GLM analysis. In each trial of the social relationship selection task, the egocentric social distance (Dso) was defined as the difference in the probability of choosing between two role names, while the allocentric social distance (Doo) was defined by the closeness between roles, measured by a questionnaire. For each subject, we built a GLM with 7 regressors (relationship selection condition and 6 head motion parameters) and 2 parametric modulators (Doo and Dso). A one-sample t-test was used in the second-level analysis to identify brain regions modulated by Doo and Dso.

Results:

Fig. 1c shows four brain regions with significant activation related to Doo, located in the visual cortex, right inferior temporal, middle frontal, and postcentral gyrus areas. Fig. 1d shows the Dso related significant activation in 13 brain regions. We also observed significant activation in the insula, thalamus, and cingulate gyrus within the brain regions regulated by Dso. The detailed information for these clusters is listed in Table 1.

Conclusions:

The current study revealed brain regions with significant activations under both egocentric and allocentric reference frames, showing that the subjects used both frameworks during the task. We also observed significant brain activations in several brain regions related to the mentalizing network and social cognition. The findings offer new cognitive and neuroscientific insights into social network expansion.

Emotion, Motivation and Social Neuroscience:

Social Cognition 2

Higher Cognitive Functions:

Executive Function, Cognitive Control and Decision Making
Higher Cognitive Functions Other

Modeling and Analysis Methods:

Activation (eg. BOLD task-fMRI)

Novel Imaging Acquisition Methods:

BOLD fMRI 1

Keywords:

Other - Social Network, Reference Frame, Allocentric Representation, Egocentric Fepresentation, Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging, Social Network Analysis

1|2Indicates the priority used for review
Supporting Image: tu_011.png
Supporting Image: Abstract_NHH_20241213_v2_01.png
 

Abstract Information

By submitting your proposal, you grant permission for the Organization for Human Brain Mapping (OHBM) to distribute your work in any format, including video, audio print and electronic text through OHBM OnDemand, social media channels, the OHBM website, or other electronic publications and media.

I accept

The Open Science Special Interest Group (OSSIG) is introducing a reproducibility challenge for OHBM 2025. This new initiative aims to enhance the reproducibility of scientific results and foster collaborations between labs. Teams will consist of a “source” party and a “reproducing” party, and will be evaluated on the success of their replication, the openness of the source work, and additional deliverables. Click here for more information. Propose your OHBM abstract(s) as source work for future OHBM meetings by selecting one of the following options:

I am submitting this abstract as an original work to be reproduced. I am available to be the “source party” in an upcoming team and consent to have this work listed on the OSSIG website. I agree to be contacted by OSSIG regarding the challenge and may share data used in this abstract with another team.

Please indicate below if your study was a "resting state" or "task-activation” study.

Task-activation

Healthy subjects only or patients (note that patient studies may also involve healthy subjects):

Healthy subjects

Was this research conducted in the United States?

No

Were any human subjects research approved by the relevant Institutional Review Board or ethics panel? NOTE: Any human subjects studies without IRB approval will be automatically rejected.

Yes

Were any animal research approved by the relevant IACUC or other animal research panel? NOTE: Any animal studies without IACUC approval will be automatically rejected.

Not applicable

Please indicate which methods were used in your research:

Functional MRI
Structural MRI
Behavior

For human MRI, what field strength scanner do you use?

3.0T

Which processing packages did you use for your study?

FSL
Other, Please list  -   fMRIprep

Provide references using APA citation style.

Butts, C. T. (2008). Social network analysis: A methodological introduction. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 11(1), 13–41. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-839X.2007.00241.x
Epstein, R. A., Patai, E. Z., Julian, J. B., & Spiers, H. J. (2017). The cognitive map in humans: Spatial navigation and beyond. Nature Neuroscience, 20(11), 1504–1513. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4656
Harris, K., & Vazire, S. (2016). On friendship development and the Big Five personality traits. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10(11), 647–667. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12287
Parkinson, C., Kleinbaum, A. M., & Wheatley, T. (2017). Spontaneous neural encoding of social network position. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(5), 0072. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0072
Peer, M., Hayman, M., Tamir, B., & Arzy, S. (2021). Brain Coding of Social Network Structure. The Journal of Neuroscience, 41(22), 4897–4909. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2641-20.2021
Schwyck, M. E., Du, M., Natarajan, P., Chwe, J. A., & Parkinson, C. (2023). Neural encoding of novel social networks: Evidence that perceivers prioritize others’ centrality. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 18(1), nsac059. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsac059
Van Den Broek, N., Deutz, M. H. F., Schoneveld, E. A., Burk, W. J., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2016). Behavioral Correlates of Prioritizing Popularity in Adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 45(12), 2444–2454. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-015-0352-7

UNESCO Institute of Statistics and World Bank Waiver Form

I attest that I currently live, work, or study in a country on the UNESCO Institute of Statistics and World Bank List of Low and Middle Income Countries list provided.

No