Common and unique network basis for externally and internally driven flexibility in cognition

Poster No:

738 

Submission Type:

Abstract Submission 

Authors:

Ziyi Huang1, Dazhi Yin1

Institutions:

1East China Normal University, Shanghai, Shanghai

First Author:

Ziyi Huang  
East China Normal University
Shanghai, Shanghai

Co-Author:

Dazhi Yin  
East China Normal University
Shanghai, Shanghai

Introduction:

Flexibility is a hallmark of cognitive control and can be driven externally and internally, corresponding to reactive flexibility (RF) and spontaneous flexibility (SF). RF refers to quick and adaptive adjustments to a changing environment, and SF refers to generating various ideas for a given topic or question (Diamond, 2013; Eslinger & Grattan, 1993). Previous work has illustrated the relevance of fronto-parietal functional networks and basal ganglia to different types of cognitive flexibility in children, adolescents, and adults (Kupis & Uddin, 2023). However, whether the development of reactive and spontaneous flexibility is mediated by the same or distinct functional networks remains largely unknown.

Methods:

Cross-sectional and longitudinal behavioral assessments and resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data of participants aged 6-35 years were collected from the Enhanced Nathan Kline Institute Rockland Sample (Nooner et al., 2012; Tobe et al., 2022). A total of 196 (95 males) and 213 (106 males) participants were selected from fMRI datasets with different repetition time (TR of 1.4 s and 0.645 s), respectively.
The RF score was obtained by averaging z-scores of trail-making test and color-word interference tasks (switching condition). The SF score was obtained by averaging z-scores of verbal fluency and design fluency tasks (fluency condition), while the reactive and spontaneous flexibility (RSF) score was obtained by averaging z-scores of verbal fluency and design fluency tasks (switching condition). Higher scores indicate better cognitive flexibility.
Nodal flexibility (NF) and functional connectivity strength (FCS), derived from dynamic and static frameworks, respectively, were used. FCS was defined as the average strength of FC between a region and all other regions, and NF was quantified as the entropy of a region's time-varying FC patterns (Yin et al., 2016). The FCS and NF were computed for each region and averaged for each cortical and subcortical (SUB) network (Schaefer et al., 2018; Yeo et al., 2011; Tian et al., 2020).
We first examined the age effects on different types of cognitive flexibility and the associations between cognitive flexibility and brain metrics. Based on that, we built nonlinear mediation models through the MedCurve toolbox (Hayes & Preacher, 2010) to explore the age-brain-behavior relationship. Furthermore, we predicted the follow-up cognitive flexibility (24-30 months after baseline visits) using the baseline brain metrics, when controlling for the baseline age and behavioral scores.
Supporting Image: fig1.png
   ·An overview of the methodology and datasets. (A) Two types of cognitive flexibility: the TMT and CWI are considered reactive flexibility-specific tasks, whereas VF and DF are considered spontaneous fl
 

Results:

(1) Quadratic effects of age were consistently observed on the RF, SF, and RSF scores (adjusted R2 > 0.26, ps < 2 × 10-13) in both TR 1.4 s and TR 0.645 s datasets. (2) Functional metrics of the ventral attention (VAN) and fronto-parietal networks (FPN) were reproducibly correlated with various types of cognitive flexibility across datasets and brain metrics (|β| > 0.13, ps < 0.10). (3) The FCS or NF of the FPN mediated the relationships of age with the RF, SF, and RSF scores at age 11 and 17 (instantaneous indirect effect [θ] > 0.03), while the FCS of the VAN mediated the relationships of age with the SF score (θ > 0.08). (4) The NF of the VAN and SUB at baseline significantly predicted the follow-up RF score (t > 2.45, ps < 0.02), while the FCS or NF of the DAN at baseline predicted the follow-up SF score (| t | > 2.08, ps < 0.04).
Supporting Image: fig2.png
   ·Mediating effects of the functional brain metrics on the development of different types of cognitive flexibility. In the TR 1.4 s dataset, the FCS of the FPN mediated the development of RSF (A) and RF
 

Conclusions:

The externally and internally driven flexibility exhibit similar developmental trajectories, and the FPN serves as the common network basis. Furthermore, our findings on the predictive roles of the VAN on reactive flexibility and the DAN on spontaneous flexibility provide deep insights into the neural substrates of different types of cognitive flexibility. Our study also suggests the importance of studying specific types of flexibility abnormalities in developmental neuropsychiatric disorders.

Higher Cognitive Functions:

Executive Function, Cognitive Control and Decision Making 1

Lifespan Development:

Early life, Adolescence, Aging

Modeling and Analysis Methods:

Connectivity (eg. functional, effective, structural)
Task-Independent and Resting-State Analysis 2

Keywords:

Cognition
Cortex
Data analysis
Development
FUNCTIONAL MRI

1|2Indicates the priority used for review

Abstract Information

By submitting your proposal, you grant permission for the Organization for Human Brain Mapping (OHBM) to distribute your work in any format, including video, audio print and electronic text through OHBM OnDemand, social media channels, the OHBM website, or other electronic publications and media.

I accept

The Open Science Special Interest Group (OSSIG) is introducing a reproducibility challenge for OHBM 2025. This new initiative aims to enhance the reproducibility of scientific results and foster collaborations between labs. Teams will consist of a “source” party and a “reproducing” party, and will be evaluated on the success of their replication, the openness of the source work, and additional deliverables. Click here for more information. Propose your OHBM abstract(s) as source work for future OHBM meetings by selecting one of the following options:

I do not want to participate in the reproducibility challenge.

Please indicate below if your study was a "resting state" or "task-activation” study.

Resting state

Healthy subjects only or patients (note that patient studies may also involve healthy subjects):

Healthy subjects

Was this research conducted in the United States?

No

Were any human subjects research approved by the relevant Institutional Review Board or ethics panel? NOTE: Any human subjects studies without IRB approval will be automatically rejected.

Yes

Were any animal research approved by the relevant IACUC or other animal research panel? NOTE: Any animal studies without IACUC approval will be automatically rejected.

Not applicable

Please indicate which methods were used in your research:

Functional MRI

For human MRI, what field strength scanner do you use?

3.0T

Which processing packages did you use for your study?

SPM
Other, Please list  -   DPABI

Provide references using APA citation style.

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168.
Eslinger, P. J. (1993). Frontal lobe and frontal-striatal substrates for different forms of human cognitive flexibility. Neuropsychologia, 31(1), 17–28.
Hayes, A. F. (2010). Quantifying and testing indirect effects in simple mediation models when the constituent paths are nonlinear. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 45(4), 627–660.
Kupis, L. B.(2023). Developmental neuroimaging of cognitive flexibility: update and future directions. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 5, 263–284.
Nooner, K. B. (2012). The NKI-Rockland sample: a model for accelerating the pace of discovery science in psychiatry. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6, 32787.
Schaefer, A.(2018). Local-Global Parcellation of the Human Cerebral Cortex from Intrinsic Functional Connectivity MRI. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y. : 1991), 28(9), 3095–3114. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx179
Yeo, B. T. (2011). The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106(3), 1125–1165.
Tian, Y. (2020). Topographic organization of the human subcortex unveiled with functional connectivity gradients. Nature Neuroscience, 23(11), 1421–1432.
Tobe, R. H. (2022). A longitudinal resource for studying connectome development and its psychiatric associations during childhood. Scientific Data, 9(1), 300.
Yin, D. (2016). Dissociable changes of frontal and parietal cortices in inherent functional flexibility across the human life span. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(39), 10060–10074.

UNESCO Institute of Statistics and World Bank Waiver Form

I attest that I currently live, work, or study in a country on the UNESCO Institute of Statistics and World Bank List of Low and Middle Income Countries list provided.

No