Decoding Sonic Logos: Exploring the physiological, psychological and musical impact on consumers

Poster No:

1928 

Submission Type:

Abstract Submission 

Authors:

Sneha Jain1, Yash Chakarvarty2, Arvind Sahay2

Institutions:

1Indian Institute of Science Education and Research (IISER) Pune, Delhi, Delhi, 2Management Development Institute (MDI) Gurgaon, Gurgaon, Haryana

First Author:

Sneha Jain  
Indian Institute of Science Education and Research (IISER) Pune
Delhi, Delhi

Co-Author(s):

Yash Chakarvarty  
Management Development Institute (MDI) Gurgaon
Gurgaon, Haryana
Arvind Sahay  
Management Development Institute (MDI) Gurgaon
Gurgaon, Haryana

Introduction:

Consumers are exposed to numerous marketing messages, but few make a lasting impression. In today's competitive environment, brands must seize opportunities and create experiences that resonate on a deeper level through multisensory marketing to tap into the brain's ability to encode and recall information. Auditory branding has been studied extensively (Krishna, 2012; Minsky & Fahey, 2017; Spence et al., 2014), but sonic logos-distinctive audio identities for brands-remain underexplored (Bonde, 2013; Gustafsson, 2015). Sonic logos are a powerful form of sonic branding, yet the elements that make them successful, particularly their ability to be memorable while conveying brand distinctiveness, are not well understood.
This study explores how brand names and sonic logos are perceived by consumers, focusing on psycholinguistic aspects and the role of sound features in shaping preferences. Using EEG and music information retrieval, we investigate the neural and behavioral responses to real and fictitious brands' sonic logos and names. We propose a model to help brands create a potent audio identity based on consumer preferences and neural activity.

Methods:

We conducted an EEG experiment with 10 participants to assess the perceptions of sound symbolism in brand names and sonic logos. The experiment involved two tasks, each with 80 trials (40 real brands, 40 fictitious brands). The tasks were presented in a randomized order.

Sound Task: Participants were exposed to brand names paired with sound stimuli-real brands with sonic logos and fictitious brands with neutral sounds. After each stimulus, participants answered two questions: "Which shape best matches the sound?" (choosing between a curved "bouba-like" shape and an angular "kiki-like" shape) and "I like this sound" (rated on a 5-point Likert scale).

Visual Task: Participants viewed brand names (real and fictitious) without audio and answered questions about the brand's familiarity and shape association (bouba-like vs. kiki-like).
Supporting Image: SL_20241219_001354_0000.png
   ·EEG experimental paradigm: stimuli design and timing of trial representation for Sound and Visual Task
 

Results:

Behavioral Data: In the sound task, we measured preference ratings for sonic logos. Real brands with bouba-like sounds received higher preference ratings compared to fictitious brands. In the visual task, familiarity with real brand names was higher than for fictitious brands.

Neural Data: EEG analysis focused on the N200 and N400 components, which reflect semantic processing and cognitive effort. The N400 component showed a larger negative amplitude for fictitious brand sounds compared to real brand sounds, suggesting greater cognitive effort for novel stimuli. Similarly, the N200 component was more negative for kiki-like sounds compared to bouba-like sounds, indicating a preference for the latter. Similar patterns emerged for the visual task, with stronger neural responses for fictitious brands and kiki-like names.
Supporting Image: SL_20241219_001318_0000.png
   ·Grand Average ERP N200 component at electrode FCz for real bouba-like, real kiki-like, fictitious bouba-like and fictitious-kiki like brand sounds
 

Conclusions:

Neural data revealed that fictitious brands required more cognitive effort to process, possibly due to unfamiliarity, while kiki-like sounds were less preferred, aligning with previous research on product preference (Telpaz et al., 2015).
Our findings emphasize the importance of sound symbolism in brand perception. The preference for bouba-like sounds was consistent across both auditory and visual tasks, suggesting that consumers favor brand names and logos associated with smoother, rounded sounds. EEG data also showed lower theta power linked to higher preference for bouba-like sounds, indicating easier processing and preference.
This research contributes to the growing field of neuromarketing, offering insights for brand development. Understanding neural mechanisms behind consumer preferences for sonic logos and sound symbolism can help brands create more effective marketing strategies. These findings have implications for creating effective sonic branding strategies and contribute to the broader understanding of how multisensory marketing can shape consumer perception and behavior.

Emotion, Motivation and Social Neuroscience:

Social Neuroscience Other 2

Higher Cognitive Functions:

Music

Modeling and Analysis Methods:

EEG/MEG Modeling and Analysis

Novel Imaging Acquisition Methods:

EEG 1

Perception, Attention and Motor Behavior:

Perception: Auditory/ Vestibular

Keywords:

Electroencephaolography (EEG)
Perception
Other - sonic branding, sound symbolism, auditory, music, brand preference

1|2Indicates the priority used for review

Abstract Information

By submitting your proposal, you grant permission for the Organization for Human Brain Mapping (OHBM) to distribute your work in any format, including video, audio print and electronic text through OHBM OnDemand, social media channels, the OHBM website, or other electronic publications and media.

I accept

The Open Science Special Interest Group (OSSIG) is introducing a reproducibility challenge for OHBM 2025. This new initiative aims to enhance the reproducibility of scientific results and foster collaborations between labs. Teams will consist of a “source” party and a “reproducing” party, and will be evaluated on the success of their replication, the openness of the source work, and additional deliverables. Click here for more information. Propose your OHBM abstract(s) as source work for future OHBM meetings by selecting one of the following options:

I do not want to participate in the reproducibility challenge.

Please indicate below if your study was a "resting state" or "task-activation” study.

Task-activation

Healthy subjects only or patients (note that patient studies may also involve healthy subjects):

Healthy subjects

Was this research conducted in the United States?

No

Were any human subjects research approved by the relevant Institutional Review Board or ethics panel? NOTE: Any human subjects studies without IRB approval will be automatically rejected.

Yes

Were any animal research approved by the relevant IACUC or other animal research panel? NOTE: Any animal studies without IACUC approval will be automatically rejected.

Not applicable

Please indicate which methods were used in your research:

EEG/ERP

Provide references using APA citation style.

Bonde, A., & Hansen, A. G. (2013). Audio logo recognition, reduced articulation and coding orientation: Rudiments of quantitative research integrating branding theory, social semiotics and music psychology. SoundEffects-An Interdisciplinary Journal of Sound and Sound Experience, 3(1-2), 112-135.
Gustafsson, C. (2015). Sonic branding: A consumer-oriented literature review. Journal of brand management, 22, 20-37.
Krishnan, V., Kellaris, J. J., & Aurand, T. W. (2012). Sonic logos: can sound influence willingness to pay?. Journal of Product & Brand Management, 21(4), 275-284.
Minsky, L., & Fahey, C. (2017). Audio Branding: Using sound to build your brand. Kogan Page Publishers.
Spence, C., Puccinelli, N. M., Grewal, D., & Roggeveen, A. L. (2014). Store atmospherics: A multisensory perspective. Psychology & Marketing, 31(7), 472-488.
Telpaz, A., Webb, R., & Levy, D. J. (2015). Using EEG to predict consumers’ future choices. Journal of Marketing Research, 52(4), 511-529.

UNESCO Institute of Statistics and World Bank Waiver Form

I attest that I currently live, work, or study in a country on the UNESCO Institute of Statistics and World Bank List of Low and Middle Income Countries list provided.

Yes

Please select the country that the first author on this abstract resides and works in from the UNESCO Institute of Statistics and World Bank List of Low and Middle Income Countries (based on gross national income per capita).

India